thanks Garrett thanks bill all for the invitation it's great to be here my first time in Atlanta seems like a great city as Garrett mentioned my lab has been interested in how the cortex represents information and processes information in the context of sensory information and we do this in the whisker system of rodents which is a really has a lot of advantages to figure out some of the key nuts and bolts for how the cortex is organized and we do many different things in this area as Garrett hinted but actually what I want to talk about today is a question that seems so simple at its outset but yet it's really complicated and interesting and that is there's a map in somatosensory cortex like in all primary cortices what is mapped what is mapped in whisker somatosensory cortex so that's what I want to talk about today so in primary sensory cortex in general I think the field feels like it's been very clear for years how this organization works and what's mapped and the main idea arises from work and visual cortex this is a classic orientation map across a little chunk of v1 where the colors show patches of neurons that are tuned for different orientations and within this map the idea supported by many many studies has been that individual neurons are tuned for local sensory features like some orientation that's produced by some specific circuit and that there's a neighborhood around each neuron that contains cells that are coding for that same feature right that's the classic mount castle cortical column from 1957 and the idea is that those columns are then topographically arranged to form these nice maps and actually an s-1 the idea has been even simpler because the main map parameter that's thought to be spread out across the cortex is simply a place on the surface of the body somatotopic location so the idea is that there are columns and a simple somatic topic map in s1 is this true while a number of years ago some experiments were done by clay reeds group which showed that if you look more closely there are more subtle and interesting aspects of these maps that really I think challenged us to understand what's really going on and two of the key experiments are shown here these are two-photon calcium imaging experiments each little blob here is a cell in cat v1 or in rat v1 and what they're doing is they're applying different visual stimuli of different orientations and then showing you in color for every single cell at cellular resolution what was the preferred orientation for that cell and here's a little chunk of v1 around an orientation pinwheel and you can see that there's a big blob of cells which like you know this orientation a big blob over here which like the opposite orientation that is we see nice orientation columns just like in the classic people and Wiesel mount Castle type models but when they did exactly the same experiments in rats in rat v1 they found this neurons are still beautifully tuned for orientation but now they're completely intermixed in a salt-and-pepper pattern and so this idea of salt and pepper maps was first discovered here but then was examined in multiple studies in v1 in many studies and a one where it's still a bit controversial but there's still there's very strong evidence for salt and pepper organization and as I'll show you an s-1 and it really seems like a salt and pepper organization is the way primary cortex is put together in rodents okay so how do we understand this rodents fundamentally different from these bigger animals how does this organization work now one interesting conceptual idea that came from studying these intermixed dispersed rodent maps is that if you think about all the cells that are tuned for one particular orientation like the yellow one they're separated in cortex but beautiful experiments by Tom MARSOC global and others showed that those cells which might be Co tuned for a particular orientation that is representing a particular input they're actually semantically linked even though they're spread apart so the idea has been that in rodents you have ensembles of cells that are tuned for particular sensory features but they're just not clustered together in a column they're spread apart and what matters is the membership of a cell and it's synaptic ensemble that's what determines its tuning not it's particularly loquacious and the analogy which I think has been very influential is if you imagine a circuit board doing some useful computation in your computer you could in principle take the locations of all the individual logical elements and you could scramble them as long as you preserve the wiring and that circuit would roughly maybe even perfectly perform the same maybe that's what's happened here in these salt-and-pepper Maps cortex so the questions that this raised for us is how to understand this is this true first of all our salt and pepper Maps an inherent property of rodent sensory cortex when you look at a map like this does this map completely lack columns which has been the idea that is the Mount Castle idea of code toon cells is that just wrong in rodents and if so a columns irrelevant for neurons sensory tuning a neuron sitting here and a neuron sitting here have very very different tuning even though they're in the same neighborhood is that really true does neighborhood have no impact if so these are the questions I want to get at so the whisker system is a great place to study this because the whiskers are these mobile tactile devices ISM sure you've heard but there's a beautiful representation and s1 cortex of the entire body map at a nice somatic topic way including in rodents a clearly definable anatomical column for each whisker on the face and that's shown here in a little schematic and we know these are real because in layer four there's actually a physical cluster of cells called a barrel that sits in the middle of a radial column and there's one of those barrels one of those columns for each whisker on the face and a perfect map okay and here's just one way that we know this is cytochrome oxidase staining here's the arrangement of whiskers on the face shown in those barrels in layer four of s1 for example for the d1 d2 d3 and d4 vibrissae okay so it's a beautifully organized place and many many old studies with classic single electrode tungsten electrode recordings showed that if you look at the average tuning of cells in a given column they look beautifully precise that is if you wiggle the d1 whisker and ask where within this overall anatomical map do cell spike they spike pretty much in the d1 column maybe a little bit around it if you wiggle d2 cell spike in the d2 column and just a little bit around it reinforcing this one whisker one-column kind of perfect topography model okay and then the last thing I'll mention is that there's an anatomical binding of instead of them which reinforce the idea that one whisker one column makes sense and that's because the lamech input representing those whisker deflections arrives in layer four and those little tight little clusters of cells and then the intra columnar circuits that originate in layer four are actually quite largely radial these layer four cells project beautifully and strongly to over lying cells in layer two three of the same column and also to the deep layers not shown here and there's lots of interesting circuits but they're very strongly linked within columns now that idea I have to say ignores important things it ignores for example in layer two through the fact that cells get lots of longer distance inputs they get inputs from nearby columns they get inputs from structures that don't map the whiskers in a topographic way or from top-down cognitive information so really there's a lot of anatomical basis to think that maybe there's some more complicated mapping but what is it and that's what I want to get at today when we think about the whisker map did we think that all the cells in the given column are tuned for the features of how that columns whisker is moving maybe position or acceleration or velocity but some kind of single whisker features or should we think about a salt-and-pepper map where those neurons are dispersed like in the v1 gate okay so we did an early study a number of years ago to get at this this is a calcium imaging study that looks at single-cell resolution at the structure of that map in layer two three okay and so Kelly Clancy who's a grad student in the lab exposed region over s1 here in a little cranial window used intrinsic signal imaging to find the column for one whisker like the d2 whisker on the face and then focused it on that column loaded all the cells with OGB which is a slightly old-fashioned but still very good calcium dye and then under anesthesia wiggled a large array of whiskers in the pseudo-random kind of sparse noise way to map the receptive fields of all those cells okay and here's what the this looks like actually with a more modern dye this is G camp oh this is really not projecting well but there's lots of pyramidal cells there we're wiggling the whiskers this is sped up quite a bit and then you can see different neurons flashing and responsive different whiskers so we can draw an analysis circle around one of these cells and then show you here in grayscale the fluorescence response of that cell over many trials of wiggling these nine different whiskers and so you can see that this cell for example loves the d1 whisker and doesn't like other whisker so this would be a receptive field map for this cell so what is the tuning for cells look like in one single column these are this is an imaging field that's localized to the center of the d2 whisker column cells are color-coded by which whisker actually drives them best and you can see that just like in rat v1 there's a salt-and-pepper intermixing of cells tuned for lots of day and this is a very very prominent thing I could point out one example these are the five most responsive cells don't number two in this field which is in the d2 column really does respond best this is Delta F over f2 the DG whisker and not to the other one so this cell is like correctly tuned for the whisker for this column but all these other cells are tuned for different whiskers okay in the summary of this study can be seen here where we localized a bunch of the imaging fields color coding the cells by their best whisker as I showed you before and we're localizing them to the actual anatomical outlines of the columns that they came from right and you can see that in each case there's salt-and-pepper intermixing but if you count carefully what you'll see is that the largest number of cells are always tuned for the correct whisker for that column about 50% of the cells the other cells are tuned for any of the number immediate neighboring whiskers and so what this means is that in layer 2 3 by this method it looks like the map is salt-and-pepper but it's correct on average just with very high local scatter okay so there's a number of important caveats to interpreting studies like this one of them is that that was done under anesthesia maybe one animal wakes up and the brain is more active maybe circuits we organized a bit and maybe there's beautiful responses that are perfectly topographic all right so we wanted to measure a receptive field map in awake animals and the whiskers this is very challenging because when animals wake up and are interested in something they actively move their whiskers around it was great for them but as experimentalist it means we can't apply a calibrated deflection to each of those whiskers like you need to to map a receptive field so how do we do this so we came up with a method to be able to do this we took advantage of the fact that mice and rats when they're in a tight spatial position they will actually rest their whiskers on nearby objects and not move their whiskers but you can show by delivering like to a moving wall panel that they can still detect and discriminate in that case so there's a passive whisker function that they can use even when they're not whisking but they're attending and so here we trained mice to be head fixed under a two-photon microscope we inserted nine different whiskers into an array of different independently controllable piezo x' and then we would wiggle either deflections of those nine single individual whiskers and the animal was trained that when we do this don't bother to lick you'll never get a reward so that's s- or no reward stimulus and then occasionally we'll wiggle all whiskers together and when we do that the animals trained lick and get a reward so the animal looks to that stimulus we also throw in some other stimuli like a couple of different tones that are not rewarded and some blanks that are not rewarded the animal learns to do this task very well here's over a bunch of training days the average behavioral response the licking of a bunch of mice to the whisk the all whisker s plus stimulus the mice learns to lick to those the mice rapidly learns to to suppress their responses to the single whisker stimuli here and then they respond even less to the tones or the blanks and the fact that they make more incorrect responses false alarms to the individual whiskers which they shouldn't then two tones means to us that they're actually paying attention to the whiskers right that's an important result so we have an animal that's receiving all these stimuli and its attentive to the whisker and what we're going to do is the animal cares about this stimulus but we are gonna care about these stimuli because here we're delivering calibrated stimuli the animal is not licking which would contaminate these responses and we're going to map receptive fields to those stimuli and what do we see so this is results from a number of mice that are all put into a common reference blank frame the black circle here is the boundaries of a reference column the center of the column is in the middle the position of each symbol is the position of a cell okay and actually these cells are all being collapsed onto exactly the same reference field so the black circle is the same column in all cases but I'm just separating out the cells the red cells in the middle are the ones that were tuned for the whisker corresponding to this column these are the correctly tuned cells right and there's lots of them but you can see that intermixed within the same population where many cells that were tuned for the next whisker over in this direction or the nest Whistler in this direction or this one or this one and in fact all the surround Wesker's and if you add these up about 50% of cells are tuned for the correct whisker and the other ones are tuned for a nearby whisker so the salt-and-pepper intermixing happens even in the awake attentive animal and this is again in layer 2 3 now if you stand back a little bit and say outside that one column is there still correct average structure you see beautifully the correct average structure now our column of focus here is just the small black circle in the middle and cells that are located outside it our position that they're correct locations outside it and so the cells that are responsive to this central whisker these guys here are not only found within that column but they're also found in the neighboring columns that's the salt and pepper map and cells that were responsive to the whisker that was one row closer to me on the face tend to be located here the cells one row in the other direction located over here etc and so you can see that the central focus of all these is topographically orderly around the central column that's the average map so the average map is correct but there's this tremendously high local scatter now I'll throw in a little fact for you that was all pyramidal cells we're doing cell type-specific imaging it's not true for every cell type one of the key inhibitory cell types and cortex is the parvalbumin or pv inter neuron and they have many interesting properties which we study in other projects but one cool property about them is that they receive input from virtually every pyramidal cell within a hundred 150 microns around them they provided inhibitory output to those same cells and so they're in a great position to average the local activity of the nearby Network so you might imagine that they're tuning might actually be more on average correct right thought that the pyramidal cells are scattered salt and pepper but these cells might be integrating and averaging and in fact that's what happens when we when we image in separate experiments in pv cream ice from pv inter neurons those neurons are actually quite precisely localized to the correct columns which with much less scatter across columns okay so the salt and pepper map seems real among pyramidal cells now we also wanted to know could it be modulated and I'm going to show you two examples of how it's going to be modulated if one is in the case of attention and really we can think about this as we measured this tuning in a trained animal that animal went through multiple days of training and during that training it's learning to pay attention to certain stimuli and not others maybe that's shaping the map right we're imagining that we're measuring some inherent property of this organization but we also know that maps are plastic maybe we've trained the map to look like this so is that true so we did in parallel with the study I just showed you another version of a task in which we applied to the awake Mouse exactly the same stimuli exactly the only thing that was different is that now the animals trained to lick to what we're presenting two tones one of the two tones and not the other and it's not looking to the all whisker stimulus so we're training the animal attend away from the whiskers a tend towards sound and particularly tone a and the animals do this they're not as good as for the whiskers but they look beautifully to the correct s plus tone they make a lot of mistakes and they they lick to the other incorrect tone as well but they looked very very little to any of the whisker stimuli or to the blanks and again this tells us that we've succeeded in the training these animals are attending to the whisker a to the sound the tones because that's the mistakes they make and not to the whiskers so how does this affect the map and here what we find is that the topography of the map is very very similar within each column this was the whisker cute cat task I showed you before here it is for the sound cute version about half the cells are tuned for the correct whisker half the cells are tuned for another whisker although actually if you notice they're slightly more cells tuned to the correct whisker in the sound cued task and this suggests that the tuning of cells within each column is a little bit more similar to each other in the sound cute case and we could analyze this in more detail and we find that this is absolutely true and that's what the bottom row shows you here so what we're computing here on the Left bar is tuning similarity between pairs of neurons we take two neurons are in our imaging field we compute what's called a signal correlation which is the similarity the mean tuning curve that's plotted here as a function of the distance between those two cells and because of the way the cortex is organized the farther apart two cells are the less similar the tuning is so there's an overall slope but you'll notice that in the sound cued case tuning similarity is higher between cells in a column these are all within a column then in the whisker cute case and that's consistent with this idea that more cells are tuned for that columnar whisker if we look not at risk or revoked activity but noise correlations which are essentially spontaneous activity that can't be explained by tuning we see a very similar phenotype that that falls off with distance as well which was known but it falls off more slowly in the sound cute case than in the whisker cute case and so what this tells us is that both sensory evoked activity and spontaneous uncontrolled activity right they're both more similar between neurons in a column when the animal is not attending to the whiskers and they're less similar within a column when the animal is tuning it is attending to the whiskers and that actually makes perfect sense because what it means is that when training and attention has told the animal pay attention and discriminate what's happening to the whiskers bells are now less correlated they're d correlated in those columns right and that provides a better discrimination signal between those different inputs so the whisker cube task D correlates neurons within each column it preserves the overall salt-and-pepper organization but it changes these subtle correlations which are in the right direction for improving coding or whisker discrimination ok the second example I want to tell you about about Kenny's maps change is more of a long-term plasticity example ok we noticed and we were a little concerned that in all of the experiments that had documented salt and pepper maps ours and s1 the v1 cases the a1 cases they were all done in animals that were raised in the normal rodent you know housing environments plucked out one day and then an imaging experiment was done but we know that sensory cortex is highly plastic the sensory experience and in particularly r23 is very highly plastic all these salt and pepper maps are layer two three we also know that standard rodent laboratory housing is actually a deprived environment there's not much going on and food falls out of the sky right the animals don't have to do very much so maybe if the animal had richer sensory experience maybe there would be that experience would refine developmentally refine these maps better and maybe they would become less salt and pepper and more columnar that is maybe salt and pepper structure is actually an artifact of how we raise our animals it's an incompletely developed map could that be so to test this another student in the lab a mule in Mussoorie a did a nice study where she took animals at the age of weaning which is just about a week after they begin to whisk and separated litters into one group that had tactile enrichment which is just toys put in the cage every couple days and the other animals that were raised in standard environments and she did this in two strains of mice that allow us either to image layer two three pyramidal cells or layer four excitatory cells and we're going to use this to ask what's the difference in topography of the map between layer four and layer two three and how does enrichment or this extra experience change that structure and this is all imaging from excitatory cells from pyramidal so before I tell you the enrichment effect let me just tell you the difference between layer four and layer two three I mentioned at the outset that layer four cells are clustered into these barrels and actually the way that Phil amic afferents innervate them is that a given Afrin essentially innervates the correct barrel and so actually you'd expect that layer four might be topographically precise even though layer two three of salt and pepper so is that true and so we normally have two animal she examined exactly that here what we're plotting is what fraction of cells are tuned for the correct whisker for the column as a function of what's the distance of that cell from the center of that column farther and farther away and what you can see in layer two three in black is that about 60 percent of cells in this study we're tuned for the correct whisker within the column and then that fell off slowly over multiple columns the columns are shown here until there's very few cells once you get a few columns away and that's consistent with the salt-and-pepper map if you look in layer four of those Minh the same normally house conditions what you find is at the center of the column in layer four there's more cells that are correctly tuned and that tuning drops off much more sharply with spacing in layer four and so what that means is layer four is in fact more topographically precise and then somehow the signals are dispersed as they move up into layer two three okay so now what is enrichment - well in Richmond actually increases the fraction of correctly tuned cells in the center of columns but it falls off approximately the same right but the height of the that tuning precision gets higher and surprisingly the same thing even happens in layer four and if you look at these Lots in the top with just some example fields I think what you can see is that after enrichment there's still salt and pepper organization but now things actually look a little bit more homogeneous okay so that's one measure of how the maps change within Richmond another measure is not thinking about salt and pepper per se at all but you can you can study the topography of the map by asking what is the point representation of a stimulus in the world in the activity of cells across that map so we wiggle one whisker and we're gonna use imaging just to ask how strong is the average response to that stimulus at one location how does that fall off that is what's the hill of activity that you see in s1 in response to that single point stimulus of the periphery okay and so what's plotted here is the mean evoked response in Delta F over F as a function of position away from the center of that column and what you can see is that in the normally has two animals you wiggle a whisker there's the strongest activity in the middle of the corresponding column and then that falls off gradually right and enrichment causes the center response to strengthen all right so the top of the hill gets taller the flanks stay the same and so the hill gets slightly sharper with it Richmond okay that happens beautifully in layer two three and not at all in layer four and I think you can see this here in the 2d version of these same plots here these these are spatial bins of cells around some reference column and the color scale shows you this strength of response to the reference whisker and I think what you can see is that when we wiggle the reference whisker and the normally housed animals yes you get activity within the column but there's a substantial spread outside the column and after enrichment that hill activity is much more focused within that average column so enrichment is sharpening up the columnar structure and actually the strongest evidence we have that the columnar structure is really sharpened comes from those measures of tuning similarity and similarity of noise correlations that I showed you before and actually in the interest of time I'm going to skip one quantification I'll just show you the other quantification okay these are spatial plots like I just showed you before or each of these histogram bins represents lots of cells that were found in a particular spatial bit and cortex relative to some reference column whose outline is shown there and the color scale in this case on the top shows you the mean tuning similarity the mean signal correlation between cells in this bin and all the cells that were found in that column so the bluer you get the higher the tuning similarity and so what you can see is that in the normally housed case many many cells over long distances have high tuning similarity to the average cell within that column and that's a reflection of the very strong salt-and-pepper intermixing that happens in those normal normally housed animals after enrichment what you can see is that there still is a ring of similarity of cells but now those are cells that are much more immediately outside the column in some distance away now the tuning similarity really drops off so you can see the development of this columnar structure and tuning similarity with enrichment and you can see that particularly well if we take this same data and instead of looking at in 2d we're just going to collapse it to one single distance dimension here along one radial dimension here which is this dimension but what's the average tuning similarity as a function of distance to that column center and here what you can see is that in the normally house case as you move in distance away from the column Center and you cross that column edge which we can see exactly we know exactly where it is from the anatomy there's very little drop-off in the signal correlation meaning tooting doesn't change much it doesn't respect the column boundaries but after enrichment tuning begins to drop off at the column boundary and the same thing is true even more strongly for noise correlations noise correlations are very strong over long distances in the normally housed cortex become become more focused after enrichment and if you look in the average here in the normally housed case again they're largely flat across that column edge but after enrichment they begin to fall off right at the column edge and so enrichment creates a tuning structure which aligns to the edges of those column boundaries okay so let's summarize this and I can tell you how how we think about these data okay so we started out with the question of how is s1 organized is every cell in each column simply encoding its corresponding whisker on the face that is their single whisker feature detectors and there are no mistakes within the map or is it some very heterogeneous salt-and-pepper organization where cells within the column are intermixed for lots of different and what I showed you is that the salt and pepper organization is very robust cost lots of different ways of measuring it but it's also plastic right and that the more experienced animal gets you can get changes in the salt and pepper organization that now begin to finally respect those column boundaries so the columns seem real but they're not perfect they're real and their edges have meanings but there still are many cells which are pepper within the salt and pepper map yeah okay and I should say so the effect of enrichment how can you think about this on circuit on the circuit level we're very interested in this but we haven't done a single experiment yet but you can ask how is it that cells within a column begin to become more similarly tuned how is it the noise correlations get actually stronger within a column but fall off a cross coat and one very simple hypothesis is that circuits within a column might strengthen with enrichment whereas circuits that go across columns might not or maybe even more inhibition develops across columns right these would both be very simple ways to produce this kind of behavior and we're very curious to know what actually happened okay so I want to take the rest of the time to answer I think a fundamental question about this there's more organization after tactile enrichment but they're still salt and pepper straw so how do we think about that salt and pepper structure and I want to boil this down to these two linked questions is the salt and pepper organization essentially noise within a distributed map that is what really matters is some kind of tuning ensemble and not the position of a cell right this cell tuned you know to the wrong whisker within the blue column was this a developmental mistake that created that cell is that self simply being averaged out and not used or is there some other type of organization that happens here and the alternative that I would propose is that maybe something else is being represented that we're not even measuring here and maybe that thing really is coherent across calm what do I mean by that when you measure a receptive field in the way that I've been describing you wiggle one whisker at a time and you're measuring the response of neurons to individual whisker deflection you get some tuning but fundamentally you're only asking how do the cells respond to single whiskers but we know in the natural case that when the animal uses all of its whiskers in the world on objects it's never one whisker that's deflected maybe there are more complicated multi whisker features which are being encoded here and maybe they're encoded in some coherent way across columns that were just missing with this organization does that make sense okay so and here's some indication that this is not so crazy so of course multiple whiskers move meet your Hartman's lab did a really nice experiment a few years ago they had essentially a vertical wall a rat you know would amble over to the wall and just whisk on it to explore it and they had a very nice clever method for being able to detect all of the individual whisker contacts that were made on to that wall by the whiskers okay and so what you're seeing here is a trace over time in black of the animals nose location it started off kind of where you guys are it would approach the vertical wall which is where the screen is it would hit the wall and then its nose started off here and lured low on the wall for a while then it started to explore higher and higher on the wall and in the meantime all of these colored dots are individual contacts by all the different whiskers on whiskers one color hitting the object okay so lots and lots of whiskers are contacting and when they add when they looked at the statistics of this which you might imagine are very complicated they were able to come away with some simple underlying first-order description the one first-order feature which is not surprising is that when multiple whiskers contact it's typically two adjacent multiple whiskers which make the closest cup right that makes sense and then number two they can analyze what's the time frame of that are the whiskers hitting at exactly the same time or at wildly different times and what they found is that most of the contacts were happening for adjacent whiskers in a plus or minus 50 millisecond interval and again that shouldn't be surprising because the animals whisking back and forth and plus or minus 50 milliseconds is basically one contact time before the whiskers retract and go forward again okay so if we want to understand how neurons and s1 might be tuned for complicated multi whisker patterns one logical place to start is in how s1 represents adjacent whisker contacts within a plus or minus 50 millisecond interval and so that's what we did the space of all multi whisker combinations is way too big to explore the tuning but there's a subspace that's tractable in this subspace that we chose here was a subspace of local that is adjacent to whisker combinations and sequences with the plus or foe but minus 50 millisecond intervals so you can imagine a bunch of whiskers on the face this would be like you know this whisker we goes first and then slightly later this one or this one first and then this one what are these if you think about the whiskers moving on objects these are essentially local motion vectors on the whisker pad okay so we want to know is there tuning in this space of local motion vectors so the experiment is a classic old-fashioned one and Mouse is anesthetized we put our nine piezas on the face we're going to apply exactly the same stimulus to each whisker but now we're gonna present lots of different stimuli and map responsiveness within this two whisker space this is an anesthetized Mouse okay and for these experiments were recording most of the time in the d1 column we Center the array on that column so that through the array we can now interleave these different types of stimuli we either present each individual with alone okay or all - whisker combinations that involve that central whisker for the column or recording I'm going to call the columnar whisker or CW - all combinations of that whisker plus the adjacent surround whiskers so CW SW combinations are these adjacent combinations that involve the columnar whisker or all the other possible - whisker combinations which are surround whisker surround whisker come and we do that in this first experiment I'll show you at just a few different Delta T's within that relevant ring and what do we see okay here's an example neuron each of these bars is the response to one of these complicated two whisker combinations stimuli here's whisker one here's whisker two the black ones are the single whisker stimuli so this particular neuron spikes best to the single whisker stimulus of the Gamma whisker and less to the d1 whisker and on and on okay but if you look at the responses to all these two whisker stimuli you see there's a bunch of them that drive stronger responses than the single whisker alone and the favorite one is the combination of gamma and d1 okay here's another cell same kind of principle except it likes a different combination it likes the e 2d one whisker coming and we analyzed when we analyzed this early experiment what we found is that virtually all cells spike more to a two whisker combination than to a single whisker so it's reasonable think cells are are liking the stimuli in some sense and when we analyze what combinations they like 70% of the cells within a column prefer one of these stimuli that is combinations that are the the columnar whisker plus a surround whisker as opposed to something more distant or something non adjacent okay and not only did they prefer those but we can make a quantitative measure of how selective they are in their tuning among either the set of B wsw sequences or the set of SW SW sequences and cells are more selective among these sequences than they are among these sequences and so that convinced us that there's something in a single column that is representing well and discriminating different two whisker combinations that involve the columnar whisker okay so we did a follow-up study to learn about this in more detail okay and so this experiment is very very similar they - five Mouse same stimuli but now actually we're going to focus on those combinations that involve the columnar whisker with just a few combinations that involve other whiskers and now we're gonna present all possible Delta T's on differences between the whiskers from minus 52 plus 50 so that we can present a very very large signal a set of thousands of stimuli we record for hours and hours and hours and then essentially we use a reverse correlation like method to go back and ask what was the favorites the meals for each cell okay so what do we find here's an example cell this cell was recorded in the d1 column and here I'm showing you in the raster's on the cells response to the d1 d2 whisker combination or the d1 c2 whisker combination etc and you can just see here that the cell likes d1 d2 also likes d1 d1 to some degree right if we blow up these raster's you can see that the individual trials here are actually trials in which were varying delta T from 50 milliseconds d2 leads d1 to 50 milliseconds in the other direction that is we're sampling that whole range and with a little bit of smoothing on this we can generate what's essentially a spatio-temporal receptive field or a stirrer for these cells in which the pairwise combinations of whiskers are on the bottom and Delta C is on the y-axis and you can see that this particular cell is this one prefers d1 d2 combination at a particular time delay of about 30 milliseconds d2 leading d1 okay so we're going to do that for all the cell's and statistically we're going to figure out which cells are significantly tuned for one particular combination over all the others and what we find when we do that is tremendous tuning for two whisker combinations in a space that I hope I can convince you looks like an intuitive real space for tuning okay so I'm gonna focus first on the spatial tuning that is tuning for the different combinations we're gonna ignore time for each cell we found the optimal delta T and this analysis was just so that delta T so here's one cell that's recorded in the d1 column the green here is the spiked count to d1 d2 combination or D 1 C 2 or D 1 C 1 and these are organized in the same layout as they are on the face ok so you can see that this cell loves d1 d2 the asterisk shows its favorite response and it responds to all the other combinations with d1 much much less now what are these other things the dashed black line is the response to d1 alone and the red line is the predicted linear response to the combinations that we applied and we're using the predicted linear response here as kind of a boring outcome if the cells responded with the same number of spikes as you'd predict from the sum of the individual whiskers we would say there's no particular computation that's going on there's not you know much happening but you can see what happens this cell responds to its favorite stimulus with pretty much the same number of spikes you would predict linearly and it responds to all the other combinations with dramatically fewer spikes that is its sharpening a two whisker tuning curve from the predicted values of its inputs okay and that's what this cell does also this cell likes b1 and b2 that's what this cell does it likes d1 and c2 this sub is the same kind of thing except its favorite response it actually manages to generate a stronger response to the favorite combination than was predicted linearly and that is what it turns out that 50% of cells in s1 do I'm just gonna flash up a number of other examples to convince you there's a lot of them the only thing I would ask you to take away from this is that all of them obey this pattern that they sharpen relative to the predicted linear sum and they're all pointing in different directions meaning that different CW SW combinations are represented in each column as kind of a basis set maybe but representing these stimuli ok so I'm gonna skip this modification but the bottom line is that we can do a number of different statistical procedures to convince ourselves and hopefully hopefully others that this tuning is real and that the tuning is absolutely sharper than the predicted linear sum and actually I'll just show you this one panel this is the selectivity of measured responses to these two whisker stimuli and this is the predicted selectivity if the cell were responding linearly the vast majority of cells sharpen their tuning and they sharpen it in a way that's represented by the example fell I showed you before they're sculpting a weaker response to all but one single preferred stimulus and the preferred stimulus they respond to pretty darn linearly okay and that suggests maybe inhibition is suppressing spikes to non-preferred stimuli but somehow allowing spikes through for preferred stimuli okay so that was about tuning for space I'll just show you one thing on tuning for time cells are in fact tuned for time difference for delta T as well that tuning can be fairly narrow this is the average response of neurons to different stimuli that have different Delta T's centered on the whatever the best delta T is for the cell and so the width of this peak is about 10 milliseconds and that means that cells have on average 10 millisecond resolution in determining which whisker went first and by how much okay and I'm not going to show you the data but most neurons like one particular tuning best they like it when a surround whisker is wiggled before the columnar whisker and not in the opposite direction not columnar before surin what does that mean in terms of local motion on the face the cells prefer local motion that's in bound to their columnar whisker okay and so what if cells are doing this and it very importantly those cells that were the pepper in the salt and pepper map that were tuned for the wrong whisker for the column they do this really well what most of those cells prefer is a combination of their favorite single neighboring whisker with the columnar whisker and so overall 70% of all cells in the column prefer a CW SW sequence as I said 50% of those missed tuned cells actually prefer that same class of stimuli and 85% of the neurons that are tuned for the columnar whisker also prefer that so there's tremendous prevalence of this type of T okay I'm gonna skip the decoding but I'll just tell you that we can what this suggests is that there are populations of cells within a column which are I diversely tuned for different CW SW sequences and therefore you can decode or the column might represent or we can decode from the population activity in the column which to whisker sequence occurred and that turns out to be quite true we can decode that actually quite well much much better than simple linear prediction okay so what is mapped in whisker s1 there's clearly salt and pepper tuning when you measure it for single whisker stimuli but what do you find if you use this particular simple simple subset of multi whisker stimuli you find that many of those cells that were tuned for the wrong whisker and single whisker space are in fact tuned for local motion from some surround whisker moving towards in an inbound direction towards the columnar whisker okay so this map in s1 is not a simple somatic topic map this map has salt and pepper organization but that organization doesn't just reflect noise it reflects something coherent and we think one thing that's being reflected here is that this organization reflects cells tuned for local multi whisker motion and a particular type we proposed at the bottom here that what each column is doing is its representing both single whisker features of course but in addition the set of all local motion sequences that are inbound to the whisker and this would be a new way of thinking about what's represented at the columnar level and I should say that there are a few very nice papers this is actually just a couple there are more that have been searching for higher-order features that are represented in s1 and there's been lots of suggestions for what these might be people have focused on very high other properties like large-scale coherent motion across the whole pad for example but what we're suggesting here is if you think on local elementary levels you can see very robust tuning across many men finally if you think about how a neuron might be constructing this narrow tuning for two whisker sequences by sculpting its response to the linear prediction this is very very similar to a computation that many of you may have seen before this is Direction selectivity in motion in the visual motion right visual motion like even in the retina neurons will receive inputs from lots of local locations in space but they might only respond when a bar is being swept in one direction and not in the other direction how do they do that they do that through a very specific and quite beautiful inhibitory circuit that's been described that suppresses the cell's response to the non-preferred direction of motion that's exactly what's going on here there's some kind of suppression we don't know what it is that's inhibiting the response of the cell to the non-preferred inputs but allowing the preferred input to get through unscathed and so maybe this is a hint that there's some kind of common sensory computation going on here in cortex there in the retina that's pulling out some useful features of the sensory world okay so how have we understood these salt-and-pepper Maps higher-order feature selectivity is intermixed in rodents and I and I would argue that's why we see this apparently this or disordered map because these cells actually are tuned for other higher-order features there is some of that feature tuning in primary cortex of larger animals as well but we think of it as taking place in downstream higher-order cortical areas maybe there's just not enough cortical territory in rodents you do that all in some higher dedicated area maybe more of that is done intermixed with in primary cortex the columns that were proposed by mount castle I would argue that they do exist despite this but cells don't share all exactly the same tuning which was his primary proposal they share related tuning features and in our case the related feature is local motion that involves the columnar whisker in some way and this suggests that columns are in fact important for computation but exactly how we still don't know okay so all the work I showed you today was done by four students Kelly Clancy super talented graduate student who now is a postdoc with Chalmers tick flow goal mu limits she did the salt-and-pepper description a mule in Missouri I did the enrichment experiment and she just finished her PhD on Chad Wang is a postdoc he did the awake experiment for measuring receptive fields in awake animals and Kevin leboy Juarez did all the work on two whisker combination tuning thank you very much yeah yeah so the question is do we also see a map of the local motion preference and actually the the primary way I would predict that that map would take place is actually within a single column I would imagine that the neurons on one side of the column let's say towards d2 they prefer d2 motion and bound of the whisker on the other side they prefer the other way that's absolutely a prediction we are gearing up right now to do the those experiments with calcium imaging but we wanted to do that in the awake case so actually we were waiting for the awake behavior to be good enough for that and it's ready now basically so that's the next thing to look at yeah far away yeah absolutely yeah we've had so the question is when we look at signal or noise correlations like after enrichment they do fall off around the central column but they often oddly pop back up again on the edges and so we've had some debate in the lab about this in the way that we're normalizing the position the normalization is quite accurate around the column but once you get far away from the column there's variation across animals about exactly which column you're in and so what we need to do is an another analysis of that data to figure out is that tuning systematically related in some way to the anatomical location of those cells and that's our hypothesis but I can't answer that for you yeah good question auditory tasks correlation prankster right yeah yeah that's a great that's a great question so what so did we do a task in which an animal was changing his attention from auditory to visual and back on Tori the whisker and back again and then we see that effect no those are actually separately trained groups of animals so we do not know yet whether that's a training effect or an attention effect however where now we've developed now a new task in the lab which is a cued attention task with the hope of getting at exactly that as you might know those are quite challenging to train and mice but I think we're getting there so I want to be able to answer that but I cannot tell you that that would be very exciting if that's true particularly because we can do you know all the cell type-specific imaging to try to figure out what's the circuit basis for that if the correlations are changing yeah yeah so is the map salt and pepper just because the brain is shrunk and there's no room to separate things out why I mean certainly some people have made that argument but I think you need to be maybe more precise in the alternative that you're suggesting so for example in the extreme you might say that maybe there's not enough cortical space in mice have separate higher-level areas and all that processing has to be intermixed in primary areas and we know that's not true right because around visual quartet v1 of mice there's an array of beautiful higher-order areas that seem to be doing higher-order feature extraction they're much smaller right than you get in primates but they do exist whether that could be true at the columnar scale the way I would phrase that is there are gonna be developmental processes and plasticity processes that help decree and shape the circuits that are building the columns right and those are gonna have some inherent spatial scale maybe the scale is such in rodents that you can't precisely wire down to that level and I think that argument you know has some merit perhaps but then there's the counter observation that like when merciful has looked for synaptic connectivity between cells that have the same tuning but are dispersed within the map there's very precise beautiful organization of that so at some level the brain is able to make very specific wiring so my gut feeling is there's something correct in the question that you're asking but so far we don't have a good or the field I don't think has a good you know mechanistic Pacific explanation for why it exists very high-level questions talked about v1 and vision Derrida hazard this conjecture about just the Madison Station in general in the skin it would be a total speculation is there salt and pepper in the skin I will say that when when people have done fairly basic experiments characterizing topography within the body map basemap and s1 like with classic tungsten electrodes that just creep along there's certainly orderly organization but it's not perfect they're already very clear from those papers that it's not perfect in fact it's less perfect than I think was suggested on average from the early whisker work so I would suspect that it occurs but it hasn't been being off or having errors whereas continuing I agree I think there's an interesting ongoing discussion about whether different cortical different sensory areas for example in rodents are all doing the same thing or uniquely different from each other and the Bill of cortex because it has those barrels seems like well maybe it's different but many many of these features are the same I would argue that these primary courtesies are very similar to each other but all cortex is a useful place to do this work because we have beautiful anatomical markers of where the column boundaries are and that makes it very possible to do this precisely but so far we haven't seen a phenomenon here which seems completely out of the ordinary for other areas but I think this is the organizational principle exploring all possible Delta T's there's a lot of overlap between them yeah yeah yeah so the way that we made the stimulus space tractable for these experiments was by not probing single whisker kinematics at all but what we did was we took a waveform for how you move away single whisker that had been determined by Dan Scholz's group using reverse correlation as like an optimal stimulus for driving s1 spiking in rats and we used that as the fixed stimulus that we never varied from whisker to whisker the only thing that we varied was which whisker was being wiggled and the delta T so it would be very interesting to know how this correlates with other types of tuning features but the dimensionality explodes and how to fit all those stimuli within an experiment becomes I think pretty much impossible well let's thank Dan